John Gribbin
Lunar Tics
Moonstruck: How Lunar Cycles Affect Life
By Ernest Naylor
Oxford University Press 229pp £18.99
Moonstruck promises far more (especially in the subtitle) than it delivers. The claims presented here for the influence of lunar cycles on life are hedged around with such terms as ‘subjective interpretation’, ‘equivocal’ and ‘little hard evidence’. But full marks to Ernest Naylor for honesty. He has produced a book that, while not exactly a light read, contains many fascinating nuggets of information.
The rationale Naylor, professor emeritus in the School of Ocean Sciences at Bangor University, offers for producing his book now is that what he calls ‘the reality of the Moon’ has been greatly enhanced by space probes and observations carried out on the Apollo missions, while at the same time its everyday (or every month) personal ‘reality’ has become obscured to city dwellers. While the science of daily (circadian) rhythms of life has advanced over the past fifty years, he says, the study of moon-related circatidal and circalunar rhythms has lagged.
But this highlights a problem with Moonstruck for the casual reader. The most pronounced lunar influences on life are created by tidal rhythms; as an oceanographer this clearly fascinates Naylor. But many of us have only a passing interest in the life cycles of crabs, marine worms and turtles, and would much rather learn about (or be entertained by stories of) lunar influences on human life. The snag is that there are precious few of them, they are mostly equivocal or subjective, and there is very little hard evidence to back them up.
All this, though, comes right at the end of the book, after the compulsory nod to ancient myths and legends and a chapter of solid science that takes in the ‘Big Splash’ model for the origin of the moon (when a Mars-sized object collided with the young earth) and the idea that life may have emerged in the tidal regions of our planet.
The intriguing nugget of information buried in the wealth of material about sea dwellers that follows is that many creatures seem to possess an internal clock, which can be set by the tides but operates semi-independently once set. The classic example is provided by a kind of worm that burrows in sandy beaches. The worms come to the surface at low tide, when the beach is uncovered, but burrow back into the sand just before the tide comes in. This can only be because their internal clock tells them the tide is due. Sure enough, when they are taken from the shore and put in tanks in the laboratory, where they experience no tides directly, they show the same rhythm of activity at first; but it gradually runs down. Then, if they are put back in their natural habitat, the body clock gets reset as they experience the ebb and flow of the tides once more.
It really is the ebb and flow that matters. There is no way that these influences can arise from the direct gravitational influence of the moon. As Naylor points out, ‘the gravitational pulls of the Moon and the Sun are so small that they affect the human body only by the weight equivalent of a bead of sweat or a human hair.’ And worms proportionately.
So, skipping lightly past the crabs and turtles, what influence, if any, does the moon have on what Naylor calls ‘the human condition’? Here we venture deep into the murky realms of equivocation and controversy. One study suggests that ruptures of aneurysms peak at the time of a new moon; another study finds no such evidence. Several published studies support the idea that the phases of the moon influence sleep patterns in people. But then again, other researchers claim that this shows up in published data because similar studies that show no such effect did not get published.
In fact, in discussing lunar influences on the human condition, Naylor is much more convincing when dismissing alleged correlations than when trying to bolster them. Contrary to popular belief, emergency admissions to hospital do not show any connection with the phases of the moon, suicides do not follow the lunar cycle, and epileptic seizures are not influenced by the moon. In 1982, a study showed that ‘there was no significant correlation between the daily or monthly phase of the Moon and human libido, thus shattering another myth concerning the Moon and the human condition’. Naylor is reduced to the rather plaintive conclusion that ‘Perhaps there may yet prove to be something behind the folklore concerning human behaviour and the Moon’; then again, perhaps not.
The biggest puzzle about Moonstruck is why (and how) it should appear under the imprint of Oxford University Press. It would be quite at home with a small press, or in this day and age as a self-published, print-on-demand title. That might also have made it possible to charge a more sensible price for what is a quite slim volume with a somewhat limited audience. But what do I know? I only write books, I don’t publish them!
Sign Up to our newsletter
Receive free articles, highlights from the archive, news, details of prizes, and much more.@Lit_Review
Follow Literary Review on Twitter
Twitter Feed
How to ruin a film - a short guide by @TWHodgkinson:
Thomas W Hodgkinson - There Was No Sorcerer
Thomas W Hodgkinson: There Was No Sorcerer - Box Office Poison: Hollywood’s Story in a Century of Flops by Tim Robey
literaryreview.co.uk
How to ruin a film - a short guide by @TWHodgkinson:
Thomas W Hodgkinson - There Was No Sorcerer
Thomas W Hodgkinson: There Was No Sorcerer - Box Office Poison: Hollywood’s Story in a Century of Flops by Tim Robey
literaryreview.co.uk
Give the gift that lasts all year with a subscription to Literary Review. Save up to 35% on the cover price when you visit us at https://literaryreview.co.uk/subscribe and enter the code 'XMAS24'